The Birth of the Biological Clock: The First Studies to Prove Our Body's 24-Hour Rhythm
From ancient speculation to Gierse's 1842 temperature measurements - how the study of human performance transformed from folklore to physics.

Jonathan Griffin
Productivity Researcher

For two thousand years, sleep and fatigue belonged to folklore. Ancient theories claimed sleep was caused by “withdrawal of blood into the veins” or an extinguishing of our “inner fire.” Then in 1842, German physician Augustus Gierse did something revolutionary: he stopped speculating and started measuring[1]Gierse, A. (1842). Quaenam sit ratio caloris organici partium inflammatione laborantium febrium vaginae in feminis menstruis et non menstruis hominis dormientis et non dormientis et denique plantarum investigatur experimentis ab aliis et a memet ipso institutis [Inaugural Dissertation, University of Halle]. Internet Archive..
His systematic temperature recordings revealed what philosophy had missed for millennia: our bodies follow a precise, predictable 24-hour rhythm[2]Piéron, H. (1913). Le problème physiologique du sommeil. Masson., p. 99. Nine years later, F. von Baerensprung discovered something even more profound: multiple body systems operate as a coordinated network, with temperature and heart rate rising and falling in perfect synchronization[3]Baerensprung, F. W. F. von. (1851). Untersuchungen über die Temperaturverhältnisse des Foetus und des erwachsenen Menschen im gesunden und kranken Zustande Archiv Für Anatomie, Physiologie Und Wissenschaftliche Medicin, 126–175., p. 161. When Italian physiologist Ugolino Mosso tried to override this biological clock through willpower alone in 1887, his body revolted with dangerous fever after just four days[4]Mosso, A. (1887). Recherches sur l’inversion des oscillations diurnes de la température chez l’homme normal. Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 8, 177–185..
These groundbreaking experiments represent key milestones in our comprehensive timeline documenting how the study of human performance transformed from ancient speculation to rigorous science between 1842 and 1889.
Table of Contents
Navigate the birth of scientific measurement in human performance
The Biological Clock: An Introduction to Our Unseen Blueprint
Exploring the century-old scientific origins of our modern productivity struggles, and introducing the circadian and ultradian rhythms that hold the key to focus
The Dawn of Measurement: The First Studies of Circadian Rhythms
From philosophical speculation to the first objective measurements of human rhythms, charting the transformation from folklore to physiology
Deep Dive: Gierse and the 24-Hour Temperature Cycle (1842)
The first systematic discovery of predictable human daily rhythms
Deep Dive: Baerensprung and the Synchronized Clock (1851)
The discovery that multiple body systems operate as a coordinated biological network
Deep Dive: Testing the Internal Clock's Limits (Mosso, 1887)
Dramatic self-experimentation reveals the power of biological rhythms over willpower
Timeline of Early Discoveries (1748-1896)
Explore the pivotal discoveries that laid the foundation for modern sleep science
Conclusion: The Foundation of Modern Performance Science
Three revolutionary discoveries that transformed human performance from folklore to measurable science
The Journey Ahead: A Preview of the Series
What's coming next in our exploration of the science of focus
Additional Resources
Supporting sections and references
The Biological Clock: An Introduction to Our Unseen Blueprint
Exploring the century-old scientific origins of our modern productivity struggles, and introducing the circadian and ultradian rhythms that hold the key to focus.
We live in an age of productivity anxiety. We chase new “hacks” and systems to manage our energy, but the fundamental problem remains: the relentless feeling of mental fatigue. We struggle for deep focus in a world of endless distraction, seeking a state of effortless performance we now call flow.
But what if the key wasn’t in a new app, but in a history we’ve long forgotten? This series retraces the science of attention to its origins to answer that question.
Welcome to the first chapter in our epic series on the science of focus.
Before we can master our focus, we must travel back to the very beginning: the revolutionary discovery of the human biological clock. We’ll establish the core concepts and define the two powerful forces that dictate our lives:
- The master 24-hour Circadian Rhythm that governs our sleep
- The faster, moment-to-moment Ultradian Rhythms that control our focus
The 19th-century discovery of these foundational rhythms is the scientific bedrock upon which modern strategies like the Pomodoro Technique unknowingly stand. Understanding their power is the first step to conquering the fatigue that defines our digital age.
This journey begins not in the digital age, but in the ancient world, with the first philosophical questions about fatigue. From there, we’ll travel to the 19th-century laboratories where the biological clock was finally, and definitively, discovered.
Our primary guide for this journey is Kleitman’s landmark 1963 review, Sleep and Wakefulness[5]Kleitman, N. (1963). Sleep and Wakefulness. 2nd ed. University of Chicago Press.. But we will go deeper, bypassing modern retellings to engage directly with the foundational science from every historical source cited here.
The Dawn of Measurement: The First Studies of Circadian Rhythms
From philosophical speculation to the first objective measurements of human rhythms, charting the transformation from folklore to physiology.
The secret to sustained performance isn’t about overpowering our biology; it’s about understanding its built-in, predictable rhythms. These ultradian cycles are the key to an ancient mystery: why some hours feel effortless, while others require Herculean effort just to concentrate. For two thousand years, the only answers to this question belonged to the realm of pure speculation, a world of poetic but profoundly unscientific guesswork.
Ancient Theories
The theories were as poetic as they were unverifiable. In the 6th century BC, Alcméon proposed that sleep was caused by the “withdrawal of blood into the veins”[2]Piéron, H. (1913). Le problème physiologique du sommeil. Masson., p. 365, a simple mechanical explanation for a profound experience. Heraclitus imagined it as an extinguishing of our “inner fire”[2]Piéron, H. (1913). Le problème physiologique du sommeil. Masson., p. 365, while the great physician Galen saw the sleep-wake cycle as a reflection of “the two phases of cardiac pulsation”[2]Piéron, H. (1913). Le problème physiologique du sommeil. Masson., p. 367.
As Nathaniel Kleitman would later summarize, nearly all these ancient theories revolved around “a shifting of the blood… either a congestion or an anemia of one part of the body or another, usually the brain”[5]Kleitman, N. (1963). Sleep and Wakefulness. 2nd ed. University of Chicago Press., p. 341. The problem, as Henri Piéron devastatingly noted, was that the evidence for these claims consisted of little more than “analogies, coincidences, and studies of comatose states”[2]Piéron, H. (1913). Le problème physiologique du sommeil. Masson., Preface. It was a science built on metaphor.
Botanical Lineage
The concept of a biological clock has ancient roots in botanical observation. “In the 13th century, Albert the Great already declared that plants slept like humans”[2]Piéron, H. (1913). Le problème physiologique du sommeil. Masson., p. 6, establishing a philosophical foundation that gained scientific legitimacy through 18th-century astronomer Jean-Jacques d’Ortous de Mairan. His systematic investigations revealed that plant movements persisted even in total darkness. As Mairan observed, the sensitive plant “still opens very noticeably during the day, and folds back up or closes regularly in the evening for the whole night” even when “always kept in a dark place”[6]de Mairan, J.-J. d’Ortous. (1729). Observation Botanique. Histoire de l’Academie Royale Des Sciences, 35., p. 35, providing the first empirical evidence for an internal timekeeper.
But this promising botanical path would prove to be a scientific dead end. By the 19th century, researchers began questioning whether plants truly “slept” at all. Henri Piéron concluded definitively that “One would therefore be right to affirm that there are no states of sleep in plants, despite appearances”[2]Piéron, H. (1913). Le problème physiologique du sommeil. Masson., p. 6. Even prominent theorists like Weismann acknowledged that any resemblance to true sleep was “entirely external, superficial”[2]Piéron, H. (1913). Le problème physiologique du sommeil. Masson., p. 9. The superficial nature of these plant analogies became clear as Piéron noted: “The so-called sleep of plants offers only superficial analogies with the hypnic states studied in higher animals”[2]Piéron, H. (1913). Le problème physiologique du sommeil. Masson., p. 6.
This systematic dismantling of plant sleep analogies, spanning from Albert the Great’s medieval declarations through Weismann’s careful distinctions, proved scientifically crucial. It forced researchers to abandon poetic metaphors and develop precise, neurological definitions of true sleep. While botanical observations had hinted at biological rhythms, their rejection as mere surface resemblance redirected scientific attention toward the genuine physiological mechanisms that would soon be revealed through Augustus Gierse’s revolutionary 1842 temperature measurements[1]Gierse, A. (1842). Quaenam sit ratio caloris organici partium inflammatione laborantium febrium vaginae in feminis menstruis et non menstruis hominis dormientis et non dormientis et denique plantarum investigatur experimentis ab aliis et a memet ipso institutis [Inaugural Dissertation, University of Halle]. Internet Archive..
When Folklore Became Physics
Then, in 1842, the world changed. A German physician named Augustus Gierse did something radical: he stopped speculating and started measuring[1]Gierse, A. (1842). Quaenam sit ratio caloris organici partium inflammatione laborantium febrium vaginae in feminis menstruis et non menstruis hominis dormientis et non dormientis et denique plantarum investigatur experimentis ab aliis et a memet ipso institutis [Inaugural Dissertation, University of Halle]. Internet Archive.. His systematic measurements revealed what two millennia of philosophy had missed. For two thousand years, the tools for studying fatigue were analogy and metaphor; in 1842, the tool became a thermometer. With a single, simple instrument, speculation was shattered by data, and the scientific study of human performance was born.
The implications were seismic. Scientists realized that this invisible machinery could not only be observed but quantified. In 1887, Ugolino Mosso’s dramatic attempt to invert his own daily schedule by willpower alone ended after just four days in a fever[4]Mosso, A. (1887). Recherches sur l’inversion des oscillations diurnes de la température chez l’homme normal. Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 8, 177–185., proving these internal rhythms were a powerful biological force.
By the turn of the century, the conversation had been transformed. Fatigue was no longer folklore; it was physics, and focus was no longer philosophy; it was physiology. This transformation was driven by a handful of pivotal investigations that dragged the study of human performance into the laboratory.
We will now take a deep dive into the most consequential of these experiments, exploring the breakthroughs that defined the era. The accompanying timeline will serve as our map, placing each discovery in its proper historical context and charting the evolution of thought throughout this foundational period.
Deep Dive: Gierse and the 24-Hour Temperature Cycle (1842)
The first systematic discovery of predictable human daily rhythms
Who Was Augustus Gierse?
Augustus Gierse was a German physician from Westphalia, born January 25, 1817, to his father Albert[1]Gierse, A. (1842). Quaenam sit ratio caloris organici partium inflammatione laborantium febrium vaginae in feminis menstruis et non menstruis hominis dormientis et non dormientis et denique plantarum investigatur experimentis ab aliis et a memet ipso institutis [Inaugural Dissertation, University of Halle]. Internet Archive., Vita. He pursued medical and surgical honors at the University of Halle, defending his dissertation on November 30, 1842, under the authority of the gracious medical order of Halle[1]Gierse, A. (1842). Quaenam sit ratio caloris organici partium inflammatione laborantium febrium vaginae in feminis menstruis et non menstruis hominis dormientis et non dormientis et denique plantarum investigatur experimentis ab aliis et a memet ipso institutis [Inaugural Dissertation, University of Halle]. Internet Archive., p. 3.
Note on Naming: Augustus Gierse has been commonly misnamed by his peers and later researchers, including Kleitman, who often referred to him simply as “Gierse” or incorrectly as “Johann Gierse.” Historical records confirm his first name was Augustus.

During his studies, Gierse received comprehensive medical training: anatomy for two semesters with d’Alton, medical clinics for four and a half semesters with Kruletberg, surgical clinics for four semesters with Blastus, and obstetrics for two semesters with Hohl[1]Gierse, A. (1842). Quaenam sit ratio caloris organici partium inflammatione laborantium febrium vaginae in feminis menstruis et non menstruis hominis dormientis et non dormientis et denique plantarum investigatur experimentis ab aliis et a memet ipso institutis [Inaugural Dissertation, University of Halle]. Internet Archive., Vita. As he wrote in his dissertation, he would “always gratefully pursue the merits of these men in my mind”[1]Gierse, A. (1842). Quaenam sit ratio caloris organici partium inflammatione laborantium febrium vaginae in feminis menstruis et non menstruis hominis dormientis et non dormientis et denique plantarum investigatur experimentis ab aliis et a memet ipso institutis [Inaugural Dissertation, University of Halle]. Internet Archive., Vita.
His 1842 dissertation, titled “Quaenam sit ratio caloris organici partium inflammatione laborantium, febrium, vaginae in feminis menstruis…” (What is the nature of organic heat in parts suffering from inflammation, of fevers, of the vagina in menstruating women…), showcased his methodical approach to physiological measurement[1]Gierse, A. (1842). Quaenam sit ratio caloris organici partium inflammatione laborantium febrium vaginae in feminis menstruis et non menstruis hominis dormientis et non dormientis et denique plantarum investigatur experimentis ab aliis et a memet ipso institutis [Inaugural Dissertation, University of Halle]. Internet Archive., p. 3. This work was awarded a prize by the medical order of Halle in a literary competition[1]Gierse, A. (1842). Quaenam sit ratio caloris organici partium inflammatione laborantium febrium vaginae in feminis menstruis et non menstruis hominis dormientis et non dormientis et denique plantarum investigatur experimentis ab aliis et a memet ipso institutis [Inaugural Dissertation, University of Halle]. Internet Archive., Preface.
The Scientific Context
By 1842, the scientific study of sleep and biological rhythms was still dominated by centuries-old speculation. Ancient theories proposed that sleep was caused by “the withdrawal of blood into the veins” or an extinguishing of our “inner fire”[2]Piéron, H. (1913). Le problème physiologique du sommeil. Masson., p. 365. Nearly all explanations revolved around “a shifting of the blood… either a congestion or an anemia of one part of the body or another, usually the brain”[5]Kleitman, N. (1963). Sleep and Wakefulness. 2nd ed. University of Chicago Press., p. 341.
In Gierse’s immediate era, the emerging field of physiology was transitioning from philosophical speculation to systematic measurement. Kleitman confirmed Gierse’s 1842 work as the “first systematic” study[5]Kleitman, N. (1963). Sleep and Wakefulness. 2nd ed. University of Chicago Press., p. 138, based on Piéron’s 1913 chronological table documenting the earliest researchers in the field[2]Piéron, H. (1913). Le problème physiologique du sommeil. Masson., p. 98.
The critical question facing researchers was both methodological and fundamental:
- Could the study of daily bodily fluctuations move beyond speculation to embrace systematic, quantitative observation?
As Victor Henri would later observe in 1895, what could not be systematically measured “belongs, not, I believe, to physiology — but to metaphysics”[2]Piéron, H. (1913). Le problème physiologique du sommeil. Masson., preface. This shift from theoretical speculation to empirical measurement represented a fundamental breakthrough, establishing that human biology operated according to discoverable laws rather than mysterious forces.
While researchers weren’t yet specifically seeking to understand biological rhythms as we know them today, they were beginning to document predictable patterns in human physiology. What transformed the field was the realization that these daily variations could be measured, quantified, and analyzed using the same rigorous methods applied to other physical phenomena.
The Landmark Experiment
In 1842, Gierse was investigating a broader question about “organic heat” in various physiological states, including inflammation, fever, and the differences between sleeping and waking states[1]Gierse, A. (1842). Quaenam sit ratio caloris organici partium inflammatione laborantium febrium vaginae in feminis menstruis et non menstruis hominis dormientis et non dormientis et denique plantarum investigatur experimentis ab aliis et a memet ipso institutis [Inaugural Dissertation, University of Halle]. Internet Archive.. His primary research focus was understanding how body temperature changed across different medical conditions, not specifically studying daily rhythms.
However, as part of this comprehensive investigation, Gierse made a methodical decision: to document how “animal heat changes at different times of the day” through careful self-observation[1]Gierse, A. (1842). Quaenam sit ratio caloris organici partium inflammatione laborantium febrium vaginae in feminis menstruis et non menstruis hominis dormientis et non dormientis et denique plantarum investigatur experimentis ab aliis et a memet ipso institutis [Inaugural Dissertation, University of Halle]. Internet Archive., p. 40. His experimental approach was remarkably systematic for the era.
Over multiple days in June 1842, Gierse conducted precise self-experimentation, taking temperature measurements under his tongue at regular intervals throughout each 24-hour period.
His original data table (one of many data sets he produced), preserved in his dissertation[1]Gierse, A. (1842). Quaenam sit ratio caloris organici partium inflammatione laborantium febrium vaginae in feminis menstruis et non menstruis hominis dormientis et non dormientis et denique plantarum investigatur experimentis ab aliis et a memet ipso institutis [Inaugural Dissertation, University of Halle]. Internet Archive., p. 40, shows the meticulous detail of his observations:

Day | Time | Pulse (per 60 sec) | Air Temp (°R) | Body Temp (°R) | Notes |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
June 21 | 8:00 AM | 54 | 15.20 | 29.60 | after the midday meal |
9:00 AM | 68 | 15.60 | 29.80 | before the midday meal | |
June 22 | 6:30 AM | 58 | 15.80 | 29.40 | after the midday meal |
1:00 PM | 64 | 19.20 | 30.00 | before supper | |
2:00 PM | 74 | — | 30.10 | after supper | |
6:45 PM | 64 | 18.00 | 30.00 | ||
June 25 | 7:00 AM | 56 | 15.50 | 29.60 | before the midday meal |
12:00 PM | 52 | 17.60 | 29.75 | ||
2:00 PM | 64 | 18.50 | 29.95 | after supper | |
3:00 PM | 64 | 18.50 | 30.00 | ||
6:00 PM | 60 | 19.10 | 30.00 | ||
8:00 PM | 62 | 18.50 | 30.06 | after a sparse evening meal |
The data revealed a consistent pattern:
Temperatures ranged from lows of approximately 29.40°R (about 36.8°C) in early morning hours to highs of 30.10°R (about 37.6°C) in afternoon periods. The Réaumur temperature scale was common in German scientific work of the era.
His pulse rates similarly fluctuated throughout the day, ranging from 52 beats per minute during quieter periods to 74 beats per minute after meals and activity.
Gierse’s Interpretation
Gierse’s methodical observations revealed what he described as a consistent and quantifiable pattern: “during the nighttime, animal heat is lower than during the afternoon” by “almost ½° R”[1]Gierse, A. (1842). Quaenam sit ratio caloris organici partium inflammatione laborantium febrium vaginae in feminis menstruis et non menstruis hominis dormientis et non dormientis et denique plantarum investigatur experimentis ab aliis et a memet ipso institutis [Inaugural Dissertation, University of Halle]. Internet Archive., p. 42. This precise measurement represented the first empirical documentation of what we now recognize as circadian temperature rhythm.
Gierse interpreted his findings through the mechanical and systematic framework of 1840s physiology. Having proven a clear pattern, he sought to explain its cause by breaking down the body’s daily functions.
He attributed the daily temperature rhythm to three primary factors:
- Muscular Activity: He argued that because “muscles are exerted much more than at night”[1]Gierse, A. (1842). Quaenam sit ratio caloris organici partium inflammatione laborantium febrium vaginae in feminis menstruis et non menstruis hominis dormientis et non dormientis et denique plantarum investigatur experimentis ab aliis et a memet ipso institutis [Inaugural Dissertation, University of Halle]. Internet Archive., pp. 42, the body’s organic heat naturally increased during the day as a direct result of this physical work. This led to his conclusion: “organic heat increases during the day and decreases at night”[1]Gierse, A. (1842). Quaenam sit ratio caloris organici partium inflammatione laborantium febrium vaginae in feminis menstruis et non menstruis hominis dormientis et non dormientis et denique plantarum investigatur experimentis ab aliis et a memet ipso institutis [Inaugural Dissertation, University of Halle]. Internet Archive., pp. 42.
- Mental and Sensory Function: He went beyond simple movement, noting that “the action of the senses and mind and emotional movements rest”[1]Gierse, A. (1842). Quaenam sit ratio caloris organici partium inflammatione laborantium febrium vaginae in feminis menstruis et non menstruis hominis dormientis et non dormientis et denique plantarum investigatur experimentis ab aliis et a memet ipso institutis [Inaugural Dissertation, University of Halle]. Internet Archive., pp. 42 during sleep, which he believed contributed to the body’s nighttime cooling.
- Vascular System Changes: His most sophisticated point involved the circulatory system, observing that “the activity of the vascular system is diminished at night”[1]Gierse, A. (1842). Quaenam sit ratio caloris organici partium inflammatione laborantium febrium vaginae in feminis menstruis et non menstruis hominis dormientis et non dormientis et denique plantarum investigatur experimentis ab aliis et a memet ipso institutis [Inaugural Dissertation, University of Halle]. Internet Archive., p. 42, creating what he called a “slightly febrile state”[1]Gierse, A. (1842). Quaenam sit ratio caloris organici partium inflammatione laborantium febrium vaginae in feminis menstruis et non menstruis hominis dormientis et non dormientis et denique plantarum investigatur experimentis ab aliis et a memet ipso institutis [Inaugural Dissertation, University of Halle]. Internet Archive., p. 42 during the day by comparison.
Gierse then synthesized these points into a single, elegant theory. He concluded that since animal heat “seems to originate from all vital functions”[1]Gierse, A. (1842). Quaenam sit ratio caloris organici partium inflammatione laborantium febrium vaginae in feminis menstruis et non menstruis hominis dormientis et non dormientis et denique plantarum investigatur experimentis ab aliis et a memet ipso institutis [Inaugural Dissertation, University of Halle]. Internet Archive., p. 42, and those functions are either resting or diminished at night, the body’s temperature must fall accordingly.
This systematic documentation of daily temperature patterns represented Gierse’s empirical contribution to understanding what he called “organic heat” across different physiological states.
What we now recognize as the first evidence of circadian temperature rhythm was, to Gierse, simply one component of his broader investigation into how bodily activities and states influenced animal heat.
Scientific Impact and Legacy
Gierse’s 1842 discovery remained largely unrecognized in his own era, a time when scientific speculation still ruled over empirical measurement[2]Piéron, H. (1913). Le problème physiologique du sommeil. Masson., p. 365. However, his systematic documentation of the 24-hour temperature rhythm provided the first objective evidence that human physiology operated according to predictable, measurable patterns.
Historical Recognition: From Obscurity to Foundational Text
The significance of Gierse’s work only became clear through later scholarly recognition. Henri Piéron’s comprehensive 1913 chronological survey of sleep research identified 1842 as the earliest systematic study in the field[2]Piéron, H. (1913). Le problème physiologique du sommeil. Masson., p. 98, confirming Gierse as the foundational researcher among all documented scientists from 1842-1900.
This finding was later confirmed by Nathaniel Kleitman:
That there is a 24-hour variation in body temperature was known for a long time. Piéron gave 1842 as the date of the first systematic study by Gierse[5]Kleitman, N. (1963). Sleep and Wakefulness. 2nd ed. University of Chicago Press., p. 138.
The True Legacy: A Revolution in Measurement
Gierse’s revolutionary contribution lay not in theory, but in his unwavering commitment to empirical measurement. His methodological manifesto reveals the scientific mindset that would transform biology:
“The proposed question was: What is the nature of organic heat in parts suffering from inflammation, let it be investigated by more accurately performed experiments. As a result, such experiments take first place.[1]Gierse, A. (1842). Quaenam sit ratio caloris organici partium inflammatione laborantium febrium vaginae in feminis menstruis et non menstruis hominis dormientis et non dormientis et denique plantarum investigatur experimentis ab aliis et a memet ipso institutis [Inaugural Dissertation, University of Halle]. Internet Archive., p. Preface
This declaration of “experiments take first place” marked a decisive break from an era dominated by theoretical speculation about “vital forces."[1]Gierse, A. (1842). Quaenam sit ratio caloris organici partium inflammatione laborantium febrium vaginae in feminis menstruis et non menstruis hominis dormientis et non dormientis et denique plantarum investigatur experimentis ab aliis et a memet ipso institutis [Inaugural Dissertation, University of Halle]. Internet Archive., p. 42 Through systematic self-experimentation and precise temperature recording, Gierse provided the empirical foundation for chronobiology, proving that daily biological fluctuations could be quantified and analyzed.
In the Pioneer’s Own Words
Augustus Gierse’s most powerful statement reveals the revolutionary scientific mindset that transformed biological research from speculation to systematic measurement:
“To know how animal heat changes at different times of the day, I made these observations on myself.[1]Gierse, A. (1842). Quaenam sit ratio caloris organici partium inflammatione laborantium febrium vaginae in feminis menstruis et non menstruis hominis dormientis et non dormientis et denique plantarum investigatur experimentis ab aliis et a memet ipso institutis [Inaugural Dissertation, University of Halle]. Internet Archive., p. 40
This simple declaration represents a revolutionary moment in scientific history. With these 17 words, Gierse transformed human performance science from folklore to physics, marking the precise point where researchers stopped guessing about biology and started measuring it.
Though he could never have imagined that his systematic approach would unlock the secrets of circadian rhythms, his commitment to empirical observation provided the methodological foundation upon which all modern understanding of human biological cycles would be built.
Deep Dive: Baerensprung and the Synchronized Clock (1851)
The discovery that multiple body systems operate as a coordinated biological network
Who Was F. von Baerensprung?

Friedrich Wilhelm Felix von Baerensprung was born March 30, 1822, in Berlin, as the second son of the then Lord Mayor von Baerensprung[8]Veit, O. (1865). Zur Erinnerung an Professor Dr. Felix von Bärensprung. Annalen des Charité-Krankenhauses und der übrigen Königlichen medicinisch-chirurgischen Lehr- und Kranken-Anstalten zu Berlin, 12(2), 74–85., p. 74. From his earliest education at the Köllnisches Realgymnasium, he showed “particular fondness for the natural sciences, especially zoology and botany”[8]Veit, O. (1865). Zur Erinnerung an Professor Dr. Felix von Bärensprung. Annalen des Charité-Krankenhauses und der übrigen Königlichen medicinisch-chirurgischen Lehr- und Kranken-Anstalten zu Berlin, 12(2), 74–85., p. 74.
Academic Formation and Medical Training: In autumn 1840, he enrolled at university, pursuing entomology, comparative anatomy, and microscopic anatomy[8]Veit, O. (1865). Zur Erinnerung an Professor Dr. Felix von Bärensprung. Annalen des Charité-Krankenhauses und der übrigen Königlichen medicinisch-chirurgischen Lehr- und Kranken-Anstalten zu Berlin, 12(2), 74–85., p. 75. At Easter 1843, he moved to Halle to study internal medicine under Professor Krukenberg, where his promise was so evident that he was appointed third assistant at the medical clinic even before completing his state examination[8]Veit, O. (1865). Zur Erinnerung an Professor Dr. Felix von Bärensprung. Annalen des Charité-Krankenhauses und der übrigen Königlichen medicinisch-chirurgischen Lehr- und Kranken-Anstalten zu Berlin, 12(2), 74–85., p. 75. After completing his state examination in winter 1844, Baerensprung spent four months in Prague studying pathological anatomy, then returned to Halle as first assistant physician from 1845-1847[8]Veit, O. (1865). Zur Erinnerung an Professor Dr. Felix von Bärensprung. Annalen des Charité-Krankenhauses und der übrigen Königlichen medicinisch-chirurgischen Lehr- und Kranken-Anstalten zu Berlin, 12(2), 74–85., p. 75. He demonstrated remarkable dedication during a devastating period when typhus and cholera ravaged Halle. After his two colleagues succumbed to typhus, Baerensprung “for a time alone under Krukenberg had to bear the supervision and burden of the inpatient and polyclinic”[8]Veit, O. (1865). Zur Erinnerung an Professor Dr. Felix von Bärensprung. Annalen des Charité-Krankenhauses und der übrigen Königlichen medicinisch-chirurgischen Lehr- und Kranken-Anstalten zu Berlin, 12(2), 74–85., p. 75.
The Temperature Pioneer: During this intense period, Baerensprung completed his habilitation on February 8, 1848[8]Veit, O. (1865). Zur Erinnerung an Professor Dr. Felix von Bärensprung. Annalen des Charité-Krankenhauses und der übrigen Königlichen medicinisch-chirurgischen Lehr- und Kranken-Anstalten zu Berlin, 12(2), 74–85., p. 76, and published his groundbreaking temperature measurement work. As his biographer noted, he “introduced temperature measurements into medicine almost simultaneously with Traube, following the example of the deceased Dr. Gierse”[8]Veit, O. (1865). Zur Erinnerung an Professor Dr. Felix von Bärensprung. Annalen des Charité-Krankenhauses und der übrigen Königlichen medicinisch-chirurgischen Lehr- und Kranken-Anstalten zu Berlin, 12(2), 74–85., p. 76. This methodical work was considered not only “epoch-making for that time, but also exemplary”[8]Veit, O. (1865). Zur Erinnerung an Professor Dr. Felix von Bärensprung. Annalen des Charité-Krankenhauses und der übrigen Königlichen medicinisch-chirurgischen Lehr- und Kranken-Anstalten zu Berlin, 12(2), 74–85., p. 76.
By 1851, Baerensprung was positioned as Privatdozent (a German academic title requiring habilitation qualification) in Halle[3]Baerensprung, F. W. F. von. (1851). Untersuchungen über die Temperaturverhältnisse des Foetus und des erwachsenen Menschen im gesunden und kranken Zustande Archiv Für Anatomie, Physiologie Und Wissenschaftliche Medicin, 126–175., p. 126, where he recognized that Gierse’s temperature discovery might be part of something far more profound than a single biological rhythm. His 1851 research paper, “Investigations on the temperature conditions of the fetus and the adult human in healthy and sick states,” represented the next crucial step in understanding our internal biological clock[3]Baerensprung, F. W. F. von. (1851). Untersuchungen über die Temperaturverhältnisse des Foetus und des erwachsenen Menschen im gesunden und kranken Zustande Archiv Für Anatomie, Physiologie Und Wissenschaftliche Medicin, 126–175.[5]Kleitman, N. (1963). Sleep and Wakefulness. 2nd ed. University of Chicago Press., p. 138.
Scientific Context
By 1851, just nine years after Gierse’s revolutionary temperature measurements, the scientific landscape was ripe for the next breakthrough. Gierse had established that body temperature followed a predictable 24-hour rhythm, but a fundamental question remained unanswered: was this an isolated phenomenon, or could other bodily functions follow similar patterns?
The critical challenge facing researchers was whether multiple physiological systems might be connected through some underlying coordination mechanism. As Henri Piéron would later observe, anything that could not be systematically measured belonged “not to physiology, but to metaphysics”[2]Piéron, H. (1913). Le problème physiologique du sommeil. Masson., p. Preface. Baerensprung would take this empirical approach one crucial step further.
It was within this evolving scientific landscape that F. von Baerensprung recognized an unprecedented opportunity. His 1851 research, titled “Untersuchungen über die Temperaturverhältnisse des Foetus und des erwachsenen Menschen im gesunden und kranken Zustande” (Investigations on the temperature conditions of the fetus and the adult human in healthy and sick states)[3]Baerensprung, F. W. F. von. (1851). Untersuchungen über die Temperaturverhältnisse des Foetus und des erwachsenen Menschen im gesunden und kranken Zustande Archiv Für Anatomie, Physiologie Und Wissenschaftliche Medicin, 126–175., was designed to answer a deceptively simple question: were body temperature fluctuations truly isolated, or did they reflect something far more profound? Could they represent a coordinated biological system operating according to a unified internal clock?
The Experiment and Methodology
Baerensprung’s approach demonstrated extraordinary methodological rigor for its time. Unlike Gierse’s self-experimentation, Baerensprung designed a comprehensive study that encompassed detailed thermometric measurements across multiple years, studying physiological conditions in both healthy and sick individuals[3]Baerensprung, F. W. F. von. (1851). Untersuchungen über die Temperaturverhältnisse des Foetus und des erwachsenen Menschen im gesunden und kranken Zustande Archiv Für Anatomie, Physiologie Und Wissenschaftliche Medicin, 126–175., p. 127.
His revolutionary approach included three key innovations:
Multi-Subject Design: Baerensprung studied individuals across a wide age spectrum, from infants to adults, providing a broader demographic foundation for his findings[5]Kleitman, N. (1963). Sleep and Wakefulness. 2nd ed. University of Chicago Press., p. 138. To systematically track developmental changes, he conducted longitudinal studies where he “determined their temperature daily, usually twice, in the morning and evening” for 20 newborn children[3]Baerensprung, F. W. F. von. (1851). Untersuchungen über die Temperaturverhältnisse des Foetus und des erwachsenen Menschen im gesunden und kranken Zustande Archiv Für Anatomie, Physiologie Und Wissenschaftliche Medicin, 126–175., p. 139, tracking patterns through childhood to puberty.
Dual-Parameter Monitoring: Most crucially, Baerensprung simultaneously tracked both body temperature and heart rate throughout 24-hour periods, creating the first systematic comparison of multiple physiological rhythms[5]Kleitman, N. (1963). Sleep and Wakefulness. 2nd ed. University of Chicago Press., p. 138.
Comprehensive Environmental Controls: The research systematically explored various influences on temperature patterns. For dietary effects, he studied “individuals who were subjected to a systematic deprivation cure due to a syphilitic ailment,” where patients were restricted to “only soup and white bread”[3]Baerensprung, F. W. F. von. (1851). Untersuchungen über die Temperaturverhältnisse des Foetus und des erwachsenen Menschen im gesunden und kranken Zustande Archiv Für Anatomie, Physiologie Und Wissenschaftliche Medicin, 126–175., p. 167. His studies encompassed physiological states like menstruation, pregnancy, and childbirth, external factors such as climate and physical activity, and the effects of blood loss through both animal experiments and human clinical observations[3]Baerensprung, F. W. F. von. (1851). Untersuchungen über die Temperaturverhältnisse des Foetus und des erwachsenen Menschen im gesunden und kranken Zustande Archiv Für Anatomie, Physiologie Und Wissenschaftliche Medicin, 126–175., p. 163.
Interpretation of the Results
What Baerensprung discovered would fundamentally reshape understanding of biological rhythms. His meticulous cross-demographic measurements revealed what he described as “complete parallelism between the average values of temperature and pulse”[3]Baerensprung, F. W. F. von. (1851). Untersuchungen über die Temperaturverhältnisse des Foetus und des erwachsenen Menschen im gesunden und kranken Zustande Archiv Für Anatomie, Physiologie Und Wissenschaftliche Medicin, 126–175., p. 161.
The Core Discovery: Perfect Synchronization Baerensprung documented this coordination with scientific precision: “The pulse rate rises and falls with temperature, and its fluctuations follow the same double wave in which the latter moves”[3]Baerensprung, F. W. F. von. (1851). Untersuchungen über die Temperaturverhältnisse des Foetus und des erwachsenen Menschen im gesunden und kranken Zustande Archiv Für Anatomie, Physiologie Und Wissenschaftliche Medicin, 126–175., p. 161. In practical terms, this meant that when body temperature reached its daily maximum, heart rate simultaneously peaked, and when temperature hit its lowest point, heart rate dropped to its minimum at exactly the same times.
The “Double Wave” Pattern Explained The “double wave” refers to the specific daily rhythm pattern Baerensprung observed, with two peaks and two valleys in each 24-hour period. He meticulously mapped this pattern: “temperature rises fairly quickly in the morning after waking and reaches a peak around the 11th hour of the forenoon [11 AM]; it then sinks a little in the following hours, until lunchtime forms the starting point of a new rise, which reaches its peak around the 6th to 7th hour of the afternoon [6-7 PM]"[3]Baerensprung, F. W. F. von. (1851). Untersuchungen über die Temperaturverhältnisse des Foetus und des erwachsenen Menschen im gesunden und kranken Zustande Archiv Für Anatomie, Physiologie Und Wissenschaftliche Medicin, 126–175., p. 161.
This created a daily pattern based on 43 systematic measurements from December 1849 to March 1850:
- Morning Peak: Temperature and heart rate rise after waking, peaking around 11 AM (pulse: 62.5, temperature: 29.88°R)
- Midday Dip: Both parameters decrease slightly in early afternoon (pulse: 60, temperature: 29.35°R)
- Evening Peak: Both rise again, reaching their highest point around 6-7 PM (pulse: 74.4, temperature: 30.1°R)
- Nighttime Valley: Both decline through the night, reaching their lowest point around 4 AM during sleep (pulse: 56, temperature: 29.0°R)
Baerensprung’s original data from his systematic study reveals the precise coordination he discovered[3]Baerensprung, F. W. F. von. (1851). Untersuchungen über die Temperaturverhältnisse des Foetus und des erwachsenen Menschen im gesunden und kranken Zustande Archiv Für Anatomie, Physiologie Und Wissenschaftliche Medicin, 126–175., p. 160:

The data clearly demonstrates the synchronized “double wave” pattern Baerensprung discovered:
Time Range | Midpoint | Description | Pulse (BPM) | Temp (°R) | Temp (°C) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
02:00-04:00 | 03:00 | Woken from sleep (2nd time) | 44.0 | 29.05°R | 36.31°C |
05:00-07:00 | 06:00 | Morning, in bed before coffee | 50.0 | 29.35°R | 36.69°C |
07:00-09:00 | 08:00 | Morning, after coffee | 57.3 | 29.75°R | 37.19°C |
09:00-11:00 | 10:00 | Forenoon | 62.5 | 29.81°R | 37.26°C |
11:00-13:00 | 12:00 | Before midday meal | 60.0 | 29.50°R | 36.88°C |
13:00-14:00 | 13:30 | After midday meal | 59.5 | 29.47°R | 36.84°C |
14:00-16:00 | 15:00 | Afternoon | 66.5 | 29.73°R | 37.16°C |
16:00-18:00 | 17:00 | Afternoon (2nd time) | 74.4 | 29.99°R | 37.49°C |
18:00-20:00 | 19:00 | After evening meal | 74.0 | 29.95°R | 37.44°C |
20:00-22:00 | 21:00 | Before sleep, during work | 67.3 | 29.62°R | 37.03°C |
22:00-24:00 | 23:00 | Woken from sleep (1st time) | 61.3 | 29.48°R | 36.85°C |
24:00-02:00 | 01:00 | Woken from sleep (1st time) | 59.6 | 29.32°R | 36.65°C |
This synchronized pattern becomes even clearer when visualized as the “double wave” that Baerensprung discovered:

The Revolutionary Insight: Endogenous Coordination Crucially, Baerensprung recognized that this wasn’t coincidence but intrinsic biological coordination. He concluded that “the undulations of temperature are typical and, although they can be modified by a change in lifestyle, they cannot be abolished”[3]Baerensprung, F. W. F. von. (1851). Untersuchungen über die Temperaturverhältnisse des Foetus und des erwachsenen Menschen im gesunden und kranken Zustande Archiv Für Anatomie, Physiologie Und Wissenschaftliche Medicin, 126–175., p. 164. This meant the body had its own internal timing system that couldn’t be simply overridden by external factors.
Scientific Impact and Legacy
The Paradigm Shift: From Isolated Phenomena to Coordinated Systems
Baerensprung’s findings transformed scientific understanding from viewing biological rhythms as isolated phenomena to recognizing them as coordinated systems operating according to a unified biological clock. The consistency across age groups suggested that synchronized biological rhythms were fundamental features of human physiology, not developmental accidents.
Historical Recognition and Legacy
Baerensprung’s revolutionary work established the foundation for modern chronobiology through several key contributions:
- Scientific Validation: Kleitman’s authoritative 1963 review confirmed Baerensprung’s crucial position in circadian science development, noting his documentation of “a coincidence of maxima and minima for both body temperatures and heart rates”[5]Kleitman, N. (1963). Sleep and Wakefulness. 2nd ed. University of Chicago Press., p. 138
- Methodological Innovation: Established the first systematic investigation of biological temperature regulation as a coordinated system
- Conceptual Framework: His demonstration of physiological synchronization suggested the existence of a central biological timekeeper coordinating multiple processes
- Enduring Insight: Recognized that biological rhythms are intrinsic, concluding that “the undulations of temperature are typical and, although they can be modified by a change in lifestyle, they cannot be abolished”[3]Baerensprung, F. W. F. von. (1851). Untersuchungen über die Temperaturverhältnisse des Foetus und des erwachsenen Menschen im gesunden und kranken Zustande Archiv Für Anatomie, Physiologie Und Wissenschaftliche Medicin, 126–175., p. 164
In the Pioneer’s Own Words
Baerensprung’s most profound insight revealed the revolutionary discovery that our biological systems operate as a coordinated network:
“A further, very remarkable result is the complete parallelism between the average values of temperature and pulse. The pulse rate rises and falls with temperature, and its fluctuations follow the same double wave in which the latter moves.[3]Baerensprung, F. W. F. von. (1851). Untersuchungen über die Temperaturverhältnisse des Foetus und des erwachsenen Menschen im gesunden und kranken Zustande Archiv Für Anatomie, Physiologie Und Wissenschaftliche Medicin, 126–175., p. 161
Deep Dive: Testing the Internal Clock's Limits (Mosso, 1887)
Dramatic self-experimentation reveals the power of biological rhythms over willpower
Who Was Ugolino Mosso?

Ugolino Mosso was an Italian physiologist whose systematic approach to self-experimentation marked him as one of the most methodical researchers in late 19th-century sleep science.
Working in an era when researchers routinely used themselves as test subjects, Ugolino Mosso distinguished himself through his meticulous experimental design and careful documentation of physiological responses.
Family and Professional Background: Ugolino Mosso was the brother of the renowned physiologist Angelo Mosso, a relationship confirmed by official records from the Italian Senate’s historical archives[10]Archivio storico del Senato. (n.d.). MOSSO Angelo. Patrimonio Dell’Archivio Storico Del Senato. Retrieved 9 September 2025, from https://patrimonio.archivio.senato.it/repertorio-senatori-regno/. He was associated with the Laboratory of Physiology at the University of Turin, where he conducted his research under the guidance of Professor Angelo Mosso[4]Mosso, A. (1887). Recherches sur l’inversion des oscillations diurnes de la température chez l’homme normal. Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 8, 177–185..
Note on Attribution: This experiment was conducted by Ugolino Mosso, who published his circadian research in Archives Italiennes de Biologie, a journal directed by his brother Angelo Mosso. Since both brothers were prominent physiologists, this family connection can sometimes create attribution confusion in historical references.
The Scientific Question: Can Willpower Defeat Biology?
By 1887, the 24-hour body temperature rhythm was an established fact[5]Kleitman, N. (1963). Sleep and Wakefulness. 2nd ed. University of Chicago Press., p. 138. But Ugolino Mosso posed a radical question: could a person, through sheer force of will, defeat their own biology? His stated goal was simple: to “displace the maximum and minimum of [his] temperature”[4]Mosso, A. (1887). Recherches sur l’inversion des oscillations diurnes de la température chez l’homme normal. Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 8, 177–185., p. 177.
This was a direct challenge to the work of researchers like Jürgensen, who had proven the rhythm was biological, not behavioral. Mosso knew exactly what he was up against, citing Jürgensen’s findings in his own paper:
“Jürgensen’s research on body temperature in the physiological state demonstrated that even in a man who remains immobile and fasting, a decrease in rectal temperature during the night and an increase during the day continues to be observed.[4]Mosso, A. (1887). Recherches sur l’inversion des oscillations diurnes de la température chez l’homme normal. Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 8, 177–185., p. 177
The Experimental Design: A Four-Day War Against the Clock
Ugolino Mosso’s plan was elegantly simple and brutally direct. The central strategy of his four-day war was to force a total inversion of his life, aiming to “completely reverse [his] occupations, so as to work only at night and sleep during the day”[4]Mosso, A. (1887). Recherches sur l’inversion des oscillations diurnes de la température chez l’homme normal. Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 8, 177–185., p. 177.
To ensure he was fighting only the clock, Mosso controlled for every other variable with scientific precision:
- Diet: He shifted his two daily meals to 11 p.m. and 6 a.m. “to eliminate as much as possible the influence of food on the chemical processes on which temperature depends”[4]Mosso, A. (1887). Recherches sur l’inversion des oscillations diurnes de la température chez l’homme normal. Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 8, 177–185., p. 177, carefully maintaining the same quantity of food throughout the observations.
- Data: He recorded his rectal temperature using a Baudin thermometer divided into tenths of a degree with a maximum indicator, consistently applying it while lying on his bed[4]Mosso, A. (1887). Recherches sur l’inversion des oscillations diurnes de la température chez l’homme normal. Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 8, 177–185., p. 178. During his working hours, he took measurements “from time to time”[4]Mosso, A. (1887). Recherches sur l’inversion des oscillations diurnes de la température chez l’homme normal. Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 8, 177–185., p. 179 throughout each day.
- Baseline: He plotted all measurements against a 36.50°C reference line “so that one could see how the elevation of my temperature gradually increased in the successive days of the experiment”[4]Mosso, A. (1887). Recherches sur l’inversion des oscillations diurnes de la température chez l’homme normal. Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 8, 177–185., p. 178.
Crucially, Mosso was also a realist, documenting the inherent limitations of his own experiment. He acknowledged that for a study with “absolute value,” he should have “condemned himself to complete rest, throughout the duration of the observations and not engaged in any intellectual work,” as Jürgensen had done before him[4]Mosso, A. (1887). Recherches sur l’inversion des oscillations diurnes de la température chez l’homme normal. Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 8, 177–185., p. 180.
He understood his approach wasn’t perfect, but proceeded anyway, knowing this self-experiment was a vital first step into uncharted scientific territory.
The Progressive Breakdown
Over four days in April 1885, Mosso documented his body’s increasingly chaotic response to the inverted schedule. His normal temperature pattern, ranging from a morning low of 36.30°C to an afternoon peak of 36.90°C, began to disintegrate almost immediately.
Day 1: Initial Resistance - Temperature remained at nighttime low (36.30°C) until 9 AM, then spiked to 37.10°C after eating. Evening temperature reached 37.20°C at 7 PM - normal afternoon levels occurring at night[4]Mosso, A. (1887). Recherches sur l’inversion des oscillations diurnes de la température chez l’homme normal. Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 8, 177–185., p. 179.
Day 2: Mounting Stress - Fell asleep at desk by 4 AM; temperature swung to 37.30°C by 8:30 AM. Woke with headache - first sign of physiological breakdown[4]Mosso, A. (1887). Recherches sur l’inversion des oscillations diurnes de la température chez l’homme normal. Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 8, 177–185., p. 179.
Day 3: Progressive Breakdown - Despite drowsiness at 5 AM, temperature registered 37.10°C, reaching 37.50°C by 7:20 AM. Body in open rebellion against artificial schedule[4]Mosso, A. (1887). Recherches sur l’inversion des oscillations diurnes de la température chez l’homme normal. Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 8, 177–185., p. 180.
Day 4: Fever Crisis - Temperature soared to dangerous 37.80°C after hospital visit. Even evening “low” of 36.80°C exceeded normal afternoon peak. Experiment terminated due to febrile state[4]Mosso, A. (1887). Recherches sur l’inversion des oscillations diurnes de la température chez l’homme normal. Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 8, 177–185., p. 180.
Below is the actual screenshot from his 1887 paper, showing his temperature recordings from the final day of the experiment; the day that forced him to terminate the study due to dangerous fever levels:

Log Entry: April 15, 1885
Ambient temperature: minimum 14°C, maximum 16°C.At 1:00 a.m., temperature = 37.10°C — 2:00 a.m. = 37.05°C — at 2:30 a.m. I start to read in bed. 3:00 a.m. = 36.90°C — 4:00 a.m. = 37.10°C — 5:00 a.m. = 37.10°C — 6:00 a.m. = 37.10°C. I dine at 6:30 a.m.; immediately after, the temperature is 37.10°C.
I then go to the hospital and remain standing until 10:45 a.m. — at 11:00 a.m. my temperature is 37.80°C — at 11:15 a.m., after 15 minutes of rest, it is 37.60°C. After about one hour of rest and sleep, I was abruptly woken up by people who had to speak to me; during this time I take my temperature, 12:00 p.m. = 37.20°C.
I wake up at 4:00 p.m. = 37.10°C; at 5:00 p.m., while remaining in bed in the dark, = 37.05°C. I have coffee, 6:00 p.m. = 37.30°C. I go home, where at 7:30 p.m. I have 37.75°C. I return to the laboratory and at 8:30 p.m. = 37.75°C. Having been at rest, at 9:30 p.m. = 37.20°C — 10:30 p.m. = 37.00°C — 11:30 p.m. = 36.80°C.
Mosso’s Own Scientific Conclusions
The complete paper reveals that Ugolino Mosso was acutely aware of his experiment’s profound implications:
An “Apparent” Inversion, Not a Real One - Mosso concluded he had failed to truly invert his body’s clock. He asked whether he had inverted the “fundamental curve” or simply superimposed a chaotic new pattern on top of the original rhythm. He concluded the latter, stating his results represented only an “apparent inversion”[4]Mosso, A. (1887). Recherches sur l’inversion des oscillations diurnes de la température chez l’homme normal. Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 8, 177–185., p. 184.
The Accumulating Physiological Cost - Mosso meticulously documented how his body’s temperature regulation broke down. He explicitly stated that his body temperature “continuously increased due to this new lifestyle, and that on the last day the temperature presented a febrile state”[4]Mosso, A. (1887). Recherches sur l’inversion des oscillations diurnes de la température chez l’homme normal. Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 8, 177–185., p. 185.
The Ultimate Warning - His most powerful conclusion captured the experiment’s core finding: “This experiment shows that one cannot invert the wakeful period with impunity… little by little an abnormal excitation of my nervous system occurred and the temperature of my body rose progressively, until it reached limits which can be called feverish, and which forced me to interrupt the experiment”[4]Mosso, A. (1887). Recherches sur l’inversion des oscillations diurnes de la température chez l’homme normal. Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 8, 177–185., p. 185.
The Legacy: How a Failed Experiment Became a Biological Law
Ugolino Mosso’s 1887 experiment, a dramatic failure in execution, became a profound scientific success. It provided the first empirical proof that the body’s 24-hour rhythms are fundamental physiological constraints, not merely malleable habits that can be overridden by willpower[5]Kleitman, N. (1963). Sleep and Wakefulness. 2nd ed. University of Chicago Press., p. 173.
The Body’s Verdict
Mosso’s own body delivered the unambiguous verdict. His fever wasn’t a random side effect but a documented physiological breakdown that revealed a non-negotiable biological breaking point. As he recorded:
“Little by little an abnormal excitation of my nervous system occurred, and the temperature of my body progressively rose, until it reached limits that can be called febrile, and which forced me to interrupt the experiment.[4]Mosso, A. (1887). Recherches sur l’inversion des oscillations diurnes de la température chez l’homme normal. Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 8, 177–185., p. 185
He was also intellectually honest about his results, concluding that he had failed to truly invert his “fundamental curve.” He termed his partial success an “apparent inversion,”[4]Mosso, A. (1887). Recherches sur l’inversion des oscillations diurnes de la température chez l’homme normal. Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 8, 177–185., p. 184 acknowledging it came at a severe physiological cost.
A Ripple Through Science
This stark demonstration of the clock’s power echoed through the next century of research:
- Influence on Later Pioneers: The experiment directly influenced researchers like Nathaniel Kleitman, who cited Mosso’s work as foundational evidence for the robustness of biological rhythms and their resistance to artificial manipulation[5]Kleitman, N. (1963). Sleep and Wakefulness. 2nd ed. University of Chicago Press., p. 173.
- Long-term Validation: While later researchers like Toulouse and Piéron proved inversion was possible over much longer periods (5-6 weeks)[5]Kleitman, N. (1963). Sleep and Wakefulness. 2nd ed. University of Chicago Press., p. 173[2]Piéron, H. (1913). Le problème physiologique du sommeil. Masson., p. 103, Mosso’s ordeal became one of several landmark studies that underscored the clock’s innate and powerful resistance to rapid change.
In the Pioneer’s Own Words
“This experiment shows that one cannot invert the wakeful period with impunity… little by little an abnormal excitation of my nervous system occurred and the temperature of my body rose progressively, until it reached limits which can be called feverish, and which forced me to interrupt the experiment.[4]Mosso, A. (1887). Recherches sur l’inversion des oscillations diurnes de la température chez l’homme normal. Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 8, 177–185., p. 185
This declaration, in Mosso’s own words, transforms what might appear to be experimental failure into profound scientific insight.
The phrase “cannot invert…with impunity” captures the essence that our internal biological clocks are not suggestions to be overridden by willpower, but fundamental requirements for survival itself.
Timeline of Early Discoveries (1748-1896)
Explore the pivotal discoveries that laid the foundation for modern sleep science
Hartley's Doctrine of Vibrations
The Finding
David Hartley published “Observations on Man” in 1748, proposing that newborn sleep patterns could be explained through his “doctrine of vibrations”; a mechanistic theory where the fetus sleeps continuously until birth brings “vigorous vibrations” from the external world.
The "So What?" (Relevance)
This was the first systematic theoretical framework attempting to explain sleep through natural philosophy, marking a crucial shift from folklore to scientific inquiry about sleep mechanisms.
"The Phenomena of Sleep appear to be very suitable to the Doctrine of Vibrations. Here I observe, first, that new-born children sleep almost always. Now this may be accounted for by the doctrine of vibrations, in the following manner: The foetus sleeps always, having no sensation from without impressed upon it, and only becomes awake upon its entrance into a new world, viz. by means of the vigorous vibrations which are impressed upon it."
Macnish's Dynamic Sleep Posture Discovery
The Finding
Robert Macnish published “The Philosophy of Sleep” in 1834, observing that sleepers naturally change positions throughout the night, challenging the prevailing notion of a single ideal sleeping posture. He argued that these position changes were caused by subtle mental activities during sleep, contradicting the idea of complete unconsciousness. Kleitman later acknowledged this contribution[5]Kleitman, N. (1963). Sleep and Wakefulness. 2nd ed. University of Chicago Press., p. 310.
The "So What?" (Relevance)
This early observation highlighted the dynamic nature of sleep, suggesting that individual preference and movement are inherent to a night’s rest rather than adhering to a rigid “correct” sleeping position. His work contributed to a more nuanced understanding of sleep hygiene and the physiological processes underlying sleep, recognizing that even subtle mental activities can influence physical posture during rest.
"I am of opinion that we rarely pass the whole of any one night in a state of perfect slumber. My reason for this supposition is, that we very seldom remain during the whole of that period in the position in which we fall asleep. This change of posture must have been occasioned by some emotion, however obscure, affecting the mind, and through it the organs of volition, whereas in complete sleep we experience no emotion whatever."
Gierse's Revolutionary Discovery
The Finding
Augustus Gierse (misnamed by his peers as Johann Gierse) conducted systematic temperature measurements as part of his study of “organic heat” across various physiological states[1]Gierse, A. (1842). Quaenam sit ratio caloris organici partium inflammatione laborantium febrium vaginae in feminis menstruis et non menstruis hominis dormientis et non dormientis et denique plantarum investigatur experimentis ab aliis et a memet ipso institutis [Inaugural Dissertation, University of Halle]. Internet Archive.. His meticulous self-experimentation revealed that human body temperature followed a precise, predictable 24-hour cycle. Though Gierse interpreted these findings within the mechanical thinking of his era, later scientists would recognize his work as the first systematic documentation of circadian rhythms.
The "So What?" (Relevance)
This landmark study marked the beginning of scientific inquiry into circadian rhythms. Piéron’s 1913 chronological review confirmed this as the earliest systematic study, making it the foundational discovery of circadian biology.
"That there is a 24-hour variation in body temperature was known for a long time, Piéron gave 1842 as the date of the first systematic study by Gierse."
Purkinje's Congestive Theory
The Finding
Jan Evangelista Purkinje proposed a “vasomotor” theory of sleep in 1846, later characterized by Henri Piéron as a “congestive” theory [2]Piéron, H. (1913). Le problème physiologique du sommeil. Masson., p. 23. Purkinje’s hypothesis posited that sleep resulted from increased blood flow (hyperemia) in the brain’s basal nuclei. This represented one of the earliest attempts to understand sleep through purely physiological and mechanical changes within the brain’s circulatory system.
The "So What?" (Relevance)
This theory marked a revolutionary shift from mystical explanations to quantifiable bodily processes. Purkinje framed sleep as a direct result of measurable changes in cerebral blood flow rather than mysterious forces. His work laid groundwork for neurological approaches to understanding rest, and Piéron’s classification within “congestive theories” demonstrates ongoing scientific debate about cerebral blood flow’s role in sleep regulation.
"Purkinje, writing in Wagner's Handwörterbuch der Physiologie, in 1846, proposed a theory, which, in a sense, is also a vasomotor theory, Piéron referring to it as a 'congestive' one. It postulated that the onset of sleep was due to a hyperemia of the basal nuclei, resulting in a compression of the corona radiata, which contains the thalamocortical tracts."
Baerensprung's Temperature-Heart Rate Connection
The Finding
German physician F. von Baerensprung expanded on Gierse’s temperature research by studying subjects across all age groups, from infants to adults. His 1851 research “Investigations on the temperature conditions of the fetus and the adult human” revealed that body temperature peaks and valleys occurred at precisely the same times as heart rate peaks and valleys.
The "So What?" (Relevance)
This discovery provided the first evidence that multiple physiological systems operate in synchronized rhythms according to an internal biological clock. Baerensprung had discovered that our bodies follow interconnected rhythms that rise and fall together, laying the foundation for modern chronobiology.
"Baerensprung in 1851, in addition to self-observation, followed the temperatures of individuals of different ages, from infants to adults, and noted a coincidence of maxima and minima for both body temperatures and heart rates."
Fleming's Carotid Compression Sleep Induction
The Finding
Alexander Fleming demonstrated that applying firm pressure to the carotid arteries could induce immediate, profound sleep. He described it as a “quick and certain” method of anaesthesia, causing unconsciousness that persisted as long as the compression was maintained.
The "So What?" (Relevance)
This was a pioneering investigation into sleep’s physiological mechanisms, directly linking changes in cerebral blood flow to consciousness. Fleming believed this discovery could illuminate the causes of natural sleep and other altered states, noting that the very “etymology of the word carotid”[13]Fleming, A. (1855). Note on the Induction of Sleep and Anæsthesia by Compression of the Carotids. The Western Journal of Medicine and Surgery, 3(6), 483–484., p. 484 reflected an ancient belief that these vessels were the key to inducing deep sleep.
"He compressed the vessels at the upper part of the neck, with the effect of causing immediately deep sleep."
Kohlschütter's First Sleep Depth Curve
The Finding
Kohlschütter published the earliest known depth-of-sleep curve, which graphically demonstrated a significant increase in sleep depth during the initial one to two hours of the night.
The "So What?" (Relevance)
This pioneering work provided an early objective measure of sleep, plotting the intensity of sounds required to awaken a sleeper at different times. This ‘classical’ curve significantly influenced subsequent research into sleep architecture, despite later methodological critiques.
"The depth of sleep, measured by the intensity of sound required for awakening, changes consistently over time according to a predictable pattern. Sleep initially deepens quickly, then more slowly, reaching its maximum depth within the first hour after falling asleep, from there it becomes progressively lighter."
Pettenkofer and Voit's Metabolic Sleep Claims
The Finding
Pettenkofer and Voit presented a report to the Academy of Sciences of Munich in 1866, claiming that oxygen intake more than doubled during sleep while carbon dioxide output significantly decreased. However, Voit later corrected these conclusions after discovering they were based on faulty calculations.
The "So What?" (Relevance)
This episode demonstrated the importance of rigorous methodology in early sleep research. While the initial claims were incorrect, they stimulated further scientific inquiry into metabolic processes during sleep and established the need for careful experimental design.
"They declared that during sleep the oxygen intake was more than doubled, while the CO2 output was markedly decreased. Voit himself found that these conclusions, based on two experiments, were due to faulty calculations."
Maury's Psychological Dream Study
The Finding
A. Maury published ‘Sommeil et les Rêves,’ a psychological study covering sleep and dreams. His work included observations on the perception of time compression during dreaming, exemplified by his famous dream of being guillotined.
The "So What?" (Relevance)
Maury’s publication provided an early psychological exploration of sleep phenomena. His personal accounts and theories on time perception in dreams were notable, influencing later discussions on the subjective nature of dreaming and its relationship to objective time.
"I feel my head separating from my torso, I awaken in the throes of the most intense anguish, and I feel on my neck the dart of my bed which had suddenly detached and fallen on my cervical vertebrae, in the manner of a guillotine blade. This had happened in an instant, as my mother confirmed to me, and yet it was this external sensation that I had taken as the starting point for a dream where so many events had succeeded one another."
Broadbent and Mosso's Periodic Sleep Breathing
The Finding
Broadbent and Mosso documented that respiration during sleep could exhibit a periodic pattern similar to Cheyne-Stokes breathing, revealing the irregular nature of respiratory movements during sleep. However, as Henri Piéron would later note, true Cheyne-Stokes breathing is “very rare” in physiological sleep[2]Piéron, H. (1913). Le problème physiologique du sommeil. Masson., p. 18.
The "So What?" (Relevance)
These early observations highlighted how the respiratory system functions differently during sleep, linking periodic breathing patterns to known physiological conditions. This nuanced understanding contributed to the developing science of sleep-related breathing disorders, distinguishing between true pathological patterns and normal sleep breathing variations.
"That respiration during sleep is likely, at times, to be so periodic as to resemble the Cheyne-Stokes type had been reported by Broadbent and Mosso in 1877-78."
Mönninghoff and Piesbergen's Secondary Sleep Peaks
The Finding
Mönninghoff and Piesbergen published “Messungen über die Tiefe des Schlafes” (Measurements on the Depth of Sleep), using auditory thresholds based on “Fechner’s principle that the depth of sleep is proportional to the strength of the sensory stimulus required to awaken the sleeper”[16]Mönninghoff, O., & Piesbergen, F. (1883). Messungen über die Tiefe des Schlafes [Measurements on the Depth of Sleep]. Zeitschrift für Biologie, 19, 114–128., p. 117. Their findings revealed a secondary peak in sleep depth and challenged Kohlschütter’s simpler model, particularly noting that peak sleep firmness occurred “only after 1 3/4 hours” rather than after one hour[16]Mönninghoff, O., & Piesbergen, F. (1883). Messungen über die Tiefe des Schlafes [Measurements on the Depth of Sleep]. Zeitschrift für Biologie, 19, 114–128., p. 195.
The "So What?" (Relevance)
This work provided the first evidence that sleep architecture was more complex and dynamic than the “classical” curve suggested. Their explicit divergence from Kohlschütter’s model, particularly regarding timing and the presence of a “morning increase” in sleep depth, laid crucial groundwork for modern sleep cycle research and recognition of varied sleep stages.
"Dr. Kohlschütter's observations differ from ours in that he observed the greatest sleep firmness after 1 hour, while according to our experiments, it was reached only after 1 3/4 hours."
Mosso's Defiant Circadian Clock
The Finding
Italian physiologist Ugolino Mosso conducted a bold self-experiment to test if willpower could override the body’s internal clock. His approach was to “completely reverse my occupations, so as to work only at night and sleep during the day”[4]Mosso, A. (1887). Recherches sur l’inversion des oscillations diurnes de la température chez l’homme normal. Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 8, 177–185., p. 177, attempting to force his temperature rhythm to flip. After just four days, his body revolted with a fever, forcing him to abandon the experiment.
The "So What?" (Relevance)
Mosso’s failure was a profound scientific success. It provided the first dramatic proof that the internal biological clock is a powerful, non-negotiable force that cannot be easily overruled by conscious discipline. His work established the serious physiological stress caused by circadian disruption, a foundational concept for all modern performance science. As Kleitman later documented, this experiment became a landmark demonstration that biological rhythms “cannot be overridden by conscious will alone”[5]Kleitman, N. (1963). Sleep and Wakefulness. 2nd ed. University of Chicago Press., p. 173.
"little by little an abnormal excitation of my nervous system occurred and my body temperature progressively rose, until it reached limits that can be called febrile, and which forced me to interrupt the experiment."
Brown-Séquard's Inhibitory Theory of Sleep
The Finding
Charles-Édouard Brown-Séquard published his revolutionary theory that both normal sleep and hypnotic states result from the active inhibition of intellectual processes in the brain’s cortex. He proposed that sleep occurs when the brain deliberately suppresses its higher cognitive functions, fundamentally linking natural rest with the mechanisms of hypnosis.
The "So What?" (Relevance)
This groundbreaking work established the first scientific framework for understanding sleep as an active neurological process rather than passive exhaustion. Brown-Séquard’s inhibitory theory directly influenced later researchers like Pavlov, who would expand these concepts into comprehensive theories of conditioned reflexes and cortical control. His work represented a crucial shift from mystical explanations of sleep to measurable brain processes.
"Concerning the nature of hypnosis, one should mention the old view of Brown-Séquard, that hypnosis is simply a matter of cortical inhibition."
Conclusion: The Biological Clock is Revealed
From philosophical guesswork to scientific reality: summarizing the discovery of the biological clock.
The experiments we’ve explored here established the foundation for understanding human biological rhythms, but they also revealed a disturbing truth: these rhythms cannot be ignored without consequence.
The transformation from ancient speculation to biological reality unfolded across generations of researchers. Our comprehensive timeline reveals dozens of incremental discoveries spanning centuries, from early mechanistic theories like Hartley’s “doctrine of vibrations” (1748)[11]Hartley, D. (1801). Observations on Man: His Frame, His Duty, and His Expectations (Vol. 1). J. Johnson. (Original work published 1749), p. 45 to Purkinje’s revolutionary blood flow explanations (1846)[2]Piéron, H. (1913). Le problème physiologique du sommeil. Masson., p. 23 and Brown-Séquard’s establishment of sleep as an active neurological process (1889)[5]Kleitman, N. (1963). Sleep and Wakefulness. 2nd ed. University of Chicago Press., p. 336. The three deep dives we’ve explored - Gierse’s temperature breakthrough[1]Gierse, A. (1842). Quaenam sit ratio caloris organici partium inflammatione laborantium febrium vaginae in feminis menstruis et non menstruis hominis dormientis et non dormientis et denique plantarum investigatur experimentis ab aliis et a memet ipso institutis [Inaugural Dissertation, University of Halle]. Internet Archive., Baerensprung’s synchronization discovery[3]Baerensprung, F. W. F. von. (1851). Untersuchungen über die Temperaturverhältnisse des Foetus und des erwachsenen Menschen im gesunden und kranken Zustande Archiv Für Anatomie, Physiologie Und Wissenschaftliche Medicin, 126–175., and Mosso’s failed override experiment[4]Mosso, A. (1887). Recherches sur l’inversion des oscillations diurnes de la température chez l’homme normal. Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 8, 177–185. - represent just the most pivotal moments in this much larger scientific evolution that transformed our understanding of human biology.
To summarise those pivotal discoveries: Researchers had proven three fundamental truths about human biology:
Our bodies follow predictable 24-hour cycles: Gierse’s systematic temperature measurements revealed that human physiology operates according to discoverable laws, with body temperature rising and falling in consistent daily patterns[1]Gierse, A. (1842). Quaenam sit ratio caloris organici partium inflammatione laborantium febrium vaginae in feminis menstruis et non menstruis hominis dormientis et non dormientis et denique plantarum investigatur experimentis ab aliis et a memet ipso institutis [Inaugural Dissertation, University of Halle]. Internet Archive.[2]Piéron, H. (1913). Le problème physiologique du sommeil. Masson., p. 98
Multiple physiological systems operate in perfect coordination: Baerensprung’s breakthrough discovery showed that temperature and heart rate fluctuations “follow the same double wave,” proving our biological systems work as a synchronized network rather than isolated processes[3]Baerensprung, F. W. F. von. (1851). Untersuchungen über die Temperaturverhältnisse des Foetus und des erwachsenen Menschen im gesunden und kranken Zustande Archiv Für Anatomie, Physiologie Und Wissenschaftliche Medicin, 126–175., p. 161[5]Kleitman, N. (1963). Sleep and Wakefulness. 2nd ed. University of Chicago Press., p. 138
These rhythms represent powerful biological forces that resist override: Mosso’s dramatic attempt to invert his daily schedule through willpower alone ended in dangerous fever after just four days, demonstrating that biological rhythms constitute an unstoppable force[4]Mosso, A. (1887). Recherches sur l’inversion des oscillations diurnes de la température chez l’homme normal. Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 8, 177–185., p. 180
The Foundation for Modern Understanding: These pioneers established the scientific framework that would eventually explain why productivity techniques like the Pomodoro method work, why shift work damages health, and why respecting our biological rhythms is essential for optimal performance.
What began with a simple thermometer in 1842 had become a new science by 1889 - one that would revolutionize our understanding of human potential and its limits.
The Journey Ahead: A Preview of the Series
What's coming next in our exploration of the science of focus
Our journey in this series has just begun. The discoveries we’ve traced here, from Hartley’s doctrine of vibrations[11]Hartley, D. (1801). Observations on Man: His Frame, His Duty, and His Expectations (Vol. 1). J. Johnson. (Original work published 1749), p. 45 to Brown-Séquard’s inhibitory theory of sleep[5]Kleitman, N. (1963). Sleep and Wakefulness. 2nd ed. University of Chicago Press., p. 336, set the stage for an extraordinary scientific adventure that spans nearly a century and a half.
Chapter 2 will take us into the brutal reality of what happens when these biological rhythms are disrupted. We’ll witness Marie de Manacéine’s shocking discovery that sleep deprivation kills faster than starvation[17]Manacéine, M. de. (1894). Quelques observations expérimentales sur l’influence de l’insomnie absolue. Archives italiennes de biologie, 21, 322-325., and follow Patrick and Gilbert’s systematic documentation[18]Patrick, G. T. W., & Gilbert, J. A. (1896). Studies from the psychological laboratory of the University of Iowa: On the effects of loss of sleep. Psychological Review, 3(5), 469–483. of how fatigue dismantles human cognition piece by piece.
Chapter 3 dives deep into the mystery of what controls these rhythms. Arthur G. Bills’ groundbreaking 1931 experiments[19]Bills, A. G. (1931). Blocking: A new principle of mental fatigue. American Journal of Psychology, 43(2), 230-245. revealed that attention itself follows rhythmic patterns. Our brains naturally “block” and recover in predictable cycles, giving us the first glimpse into the biological machinery of focus.
The remainder of the series brings us to the ultimate tests of human biological limits and the synthesis of this groundbreaking science. We’ll descend 150 feet underground with Nathaniel Kleitman during his legendary 32-day experiment in Mammoth Cave, then follow him to the Norwegian Arctic[20]Kleitman, N., & Kleitman, E. (1953). Effect of Non-Twenty-Four-Hour Routines of Living on Oral Temperature and Heart Rate. Journal of Applied Physiology, 6(5), 283–291. where the sun never sets. Both experiments were audacious attempts to override millions of years of evolution. We’ll witness David Tyler’s neurophysiological discoveries[21]Tyler, D. B., Goodman, J., & Rothman, T. (1947). THE EFFECT OF EXPERIMENTAL INSOMNIA ON THE RATE OF POTENTIAL CHANGES IN THE BRAIN. American Journal of Physiology-Legacy Content, 149(1), 185–193. that revealed the true biological cost of mental effort, and explore Kleitman’s monumental synthesis in “Sleep and Wakefulness”[5]Kleitman, N. (1963). Sleep and Wakefulness. 2nd ed. University of Chicago Press., the work that established sleep research as a legitimate science.
Our story culminates with Kleitman’s groundbreaking 1982 follow-up research on “Basic Rest-Activity Cycles”[22]Kleitman, N. (1982). Basic Rest-Activity Cycle—22 Years Later. Sleep, 5(4), 311–317., which revealed the 90-minute ultradian rhythms that govern our optimal focus periods. These are the very rhythms that explain why the Pomodoro Technique works.
This is the story of how science transformed our understanding of human performance from folklore to physics, and how these discoveries hold the key to mastering focus in our modern world.
References
Academic sources and historical research citations
- 1.Gierse, A. (1842). Quaenam sit ratio caloris organici partium inflammatione laborantium febrium vaginae in feminis menstruis et non menstruis hominis dormientis et non dormientis et denique plantarum investigatur experimentis ab aliis et a memet ipso institutis [Inaugural Dissertation, University of Halle]. Internet Archive. (Cited on Vita, p. 3, Preface, p. 40, p. 42, pp. 42, and p. Preface)
- 2.Piéron, H. (1913). Le problème physiologique du sommeil. Masson. (Cited on p. 99, p. 365, p. 367, Preface, p. 6, p. 9, p. 98, preface, p. Preface, p. 103, p. 23, and p. 18)
- 3.Baerensprung, F. W. F. von. (1851). Untersuchungen über die Temperaturverhältnisse des Foetus und des erwachsenen Menschen im gesunden und kranken Zustande Archiv Für Anatomie, Physiologie Und Wissenschaftliche Medicin, 126–175. (Cited on p. 161, p. 126, p. 127, p. 139, p. 167, p. 163, p. 160, and p. 164)
- 4.Mosso, A. (1887). Recherches sur l'inversion des oscillations diurnes de la température chez l'homme normal. Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 8, 177–185. (Cited on p. 177, p. 178, p. 179, p. 180, p. 184, and p. 185)
- 5.Kleitman, N. (1963). Sleep and Wakefulness. 2nd ed. University of Chicago Press. (Cited on p. 341, p. 138, p. 173, p. 310, p. 347, p. 51, p. 49, and p. 336)
- 6.de Mairan, J.-J. d'Ortous. (1729). Observation Botanique. Histoire de l'Academie Royale Des Sciences, 35. (Cited on p. 35)
- 7.Ehring, F. (1989). Hautkrankheiten: 5 Jahrhunderte wissenschaftlicher Illustration = Skin diseases. Fischer.
- 8.Veit, O. (1865). Zur Erinnerung an Professor Dr. Felix von Bärensprung. Annalen des Charité-Krankenhauses und der übrigen Königlichen medicinisch-chirurgischen Lehr- und Kranken-Anstalten zu Berlin, 12(2), 74–85. (Cited on p. 74, p. 75, and p. 76)
- 9.Fausone, M., & Galloni, M. (2018). Angelo Mosso: Transduction and Measurement of Physiological Signals. ACTA IMEKO, 7(2), 110.
- 10.Archivio storico del Senato. (n.d.). MOSSO Angelo. Patrimonio Dell'Archivio Storico Del Senato. Retrieved 9 September 2025, from https://patrimonio.archivio.senato.it/repertorio-senatori-regno/
- 11.Hartley, D. (1801). Observations on Man: His Frame, His Duty, and His Expectations (Vol. 1). J. Johnson. (Original work published 1749) (Cited on p. 45)
- 12.Macnish, R. (1834). The philosophy of sleep (2nd ed.). W. R. M'Phun. (Cited on p. 24)
- 13.Fleming, A. (1855). Note on the Induction of Sleep and Anæsthesia by Compression of the Carotids. The Western Journal of Medicine and Surgery, 3(6), 483–484. (Cited on p. 484, and p. 483)
- 14.Kohlschütter, E. O. H. (1863). Messungen der Festigkeit des Schlafes. Zeitschrift für rationelle Medicin, 17, 209–253. (Cited on p. 252)
- 15.
- 16.Mönninghoff, O., & Piesbergen, F. (1883). Messungen über die Tiefe des Schlafes [Measurements on the Depth of Sleep]. Zeitschrift für Biologie, 19, 114–128. (Cited on p. 117, and p. 195)
- 17.Manacéine, M. de. (1894). Quelques observations expérimentales sur l'influence de l'insomnie absolue. Archives italiennes de biologie, 21, 322-325.
- 18.Patrick, G. T. W., & Gilbert, J. A. (1896). Studies from the psychological laboratory of the University of Iowa: On the effects of loss of sleep. Psychological Review, 3(5), 469–483.
- 19.Bills, A. G. (1931). Blocking: A new principle of mental fatigue. American Journal of Psychology, 43(2), 230-245.
- 20.Kleitman, N., & Kleitman, E. (1953). Effect of Non-Twenty-Four-Hour Routines of Living on Oral Temperature and Heart Rate. Journal of Applied Physiology, 6(5), 283–291.
- 21.Tyler, D. B., Goodman, J., & Rothman, T. (1947). THE EFFECT OF EXPERIMENTAL INSOMNIA ON THE RATE OF POTENTIAL CHANGES IN THE BRAIN. American Journal of Physiology-Legacy Content, 149(1), 185–193.
- 22.Kleitman, N. (1982). Basic Rest-Activity Cycle—22 Years Later. Sleep, 5(4), 311–317.
Season 1 Articles
Explore all articles in this season

The Birth of the Biological Clock: The First Studies to Prove Our Body's 24-Hour Rhythm
From ancient speculation to Gierse's 1842 temperature measurements - how the study of human performance transformed from …